

Takeo Fujiwara1,2*, Manami Ochi1,2 and Maiko Osawa1
*Correspondence: Takeo Fujiwara fujiwara-tk@ncchd.go.jp
1. Department of Social Medicine, National Research Institute for Child Health and Development, Tokyo, Japan.
2. Department of Developmental Social Medicine, Mie University Graduate School of Medicine, Mie, Japan.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Background: Children with poor communication skills, such as autism, are known to have lower levels of oxytocin. However, reports on the association between social behavior and oxytocin levels in children are scarce. Similarly, few studies have shown the association between child temperament and oxytocin levels. The purpose of this study was to investigate the association of child temperament and social behavior with levels of oxytocin.
Methods: Urine samples from 23 healthy Japanese children (13 boys, 10 girls) aged 23–48 months were used to measure oxytocin levels. Temperament and behavior were assessed using the Toddler Temperament Scale (TTS) and the Strength and Difficulty Questionnaire (SDQ), respectively. The association of temperament and behavior with levels of oxytocin were analyzed.
Results: A child's persistence temperament was significantly inversely associated with oxytocin (Pearson's correlation (r)=−0.52, p=0.01), and remained significant even after adjusted for covariates (partial correlation=−0.63, p=0.007). Among SDQ subscales, peer problems were inversely associated (r=−0.60, p=0.002), while prosocial behavior was positively associated (r=0.53, p=0.01) with oxytocin, although the associations became weaker after adjusting for covariates. Other TTS and SDQ subscales were not associated with oxytocin.
Conclusion: A child's persistence temperament and behavior related to peer problems were inversely associated with oxytocin levels, while prosocial behavior was positively associated with oxytocin levels. Further research is needed to elucidate the mechanism of child temperament and behavior via oxytocin.
Keywords: Oxytocin, temperament, prosocial behavior, difficult behavior, toddlers, autistic spectrum disorders
Oxytocin (OT) is known to be associated with social behavior [1,2], including romantic love [3,4], parenting [5-7], and trust [8,9] in adults. Other studies have investigated the association between social behavior and circulating OT levels among children with autism spectrum disorders [10,11] or schizophrenia [12]. Further, several studies reported the effectiveness of intranasal application of OT to improve social behavior among autism spectrum disorders or schizophrenia, in which social behavior is a central role [13-17]. However, to the best of our knowledge, few studies have investigated the association between OT and social behaviors among healthy children, especially toddlers.
As social behavior is associated with OT or related genes [10,11,18-21], it is reasonable to hypothesize that temperament might be associated with OT levels because of the robust association between temperament and behavior [22]. Thomas and Chess were aware of the nine dimensions of temperament, which include activity, regularity, initial reaction, adaptability, intensity, mood, distractibility, persistence, attention span and sensitivity, which they based their interviews with parents of infants on [23]. Meanwhile, Rothbart and Derryberry defined temperament as individual differences in emotional, motor and attentional reactivity [24], with these biologically based differences linked to an individual's genetic endowment [25]. Recent studies have revealed that oxytocin-receptor gene variations were associated with autism spectrum disorders [18,19] and children's aggressive behavior [20,21]. Based on that, certain genetic components related to temperament may be associated with OT-related genetic components [7,18,19], and studying them could provide further insight into the developmental mechanism of temperament. Although limited to adult samples, Tost et al., have in fact reported that a common allele in the OT receptor gene was associated with prosocial temperament [26]. In order to avoid the influence of environmental factors on temperament, it is necessary to investigate the association between temperament and OT levels in children of a young age, such as toddlers.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the association of temperament and social behavior with oxytocin levels among toddlers.
Participants
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the National Institute for Public Health. All participants signed informed consent forms prior to enrollment in the study. 52 mothers with 18–48-month-old children were recruited through snowball sampling (from personal connections of research coordinators or the participants themselves). All participants were Japanese living in greater Tokyo. Urine samples were successfully collected from 23 healthy children with no diagnosed developmental disability.
OT level analysis
Research coordinators visited each participant's home for approximately an hour between 11 a.m. and 2 p.m. Urine samples were obtained from the children as the collection procedure is noninvasive and easy to conduct. A 1 mL urine sample from each child was collected in a tube to which a 40 µL aliquot of sodium citrate buffer (0.03 M sodium citrate, 25 mM EDTA and 0.35 mM 1,10-phenanthroline) was added. Samples were stored immediately in a cooler box at 4°C for a maximum of two hours, and later kept at −20°C in the laboratory.
OT concentrations in the urine samples were measured by a competitive radioimmunoassay as described elsewhere [27]. Measurement of urinary OT per mg of creatinine showed a strong correlation with plasma OT measurements in a previous study (r=0.89) [28]. Briefly, rabbit antiserum specific for human OT was generated by immunizing a rabbit four times with recombinant human OT (ASKA Pharmaceutical. Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) conjugated with water-soluble carbodiimide (Nakarai Tesque, Tokyo, Japan). Urine samples were decomplemented at 56°C for 30 min, with the supernatant extracted after centrifugation (3,000 rpm, 10 min, 4°C). An equal amount of decomplemented sample and 125I-labeled OT (Perkin Elmer Life Sciences, Inc., Boston MA) solution were allocated for use in an assay tube (Shionogi, Tokyo, Japan). Rabbit anti-OT serum was then added to each assay tube followed by incubation for two days at 4°C. Subsequently, goat anti-rabbit IgG serum (ASKA Pharmaceutical. Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) was added to each assay tube followed by incubation for one day at 4°C. After centrifugation, radioreactivity of the pellet was measured by a gamma counter (Auto Well Gamma System ARC-1000M, Aloka, Tokyo Japan). According to a standard curve, the minimal detection limit of the assay was 3 µU/mL (1 µU of OT is equivalent to 1.776 pg). All assays were performed in duplicate. The assay's intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variability were <14.2%. The concentration of OT in the urine was standardized according to the urinary creatinine concentration. Urinary creatinine was measured using the alkaline picrate colorimetric method (modified Jaffe).
Statistical analysis
TTS and SDQ score calculation were performed before obtaining the OT result, eliminating the influence by the results on OT levels. First, the associations between demographic, psychosocial, environmental and familial characteristics (age, sex, number of siblings, attendance of preschool or day care, having a friend to play with regularly, and annual household income), and OT levels were investigated using t-test analysis. Further, associations of the TTS and SDQ subscales with urinary OT levels were analyzed using Pearson's correlation, including bivariate models to determine both the crude association and partial correlation while excluding the effect of covariates (i.e., controlled for environmental and familial characteristics). All analyses were performed using the STATA MP version 12.0 software package (STATA Corporation, College Station,
TX 2011).
Characteristics of the children
(Table 1) The mean age was 38.3 months (standard deviation [SD], 6.2; range, 23–48 months). Sex ratio was almost identical (13 boys; 56.5%). The mean number of siblings was 0.7 (SD, 0.8); more precisely, there were no siblings in 12 cases (52.2%), one sibling in seven cases (30.4%), and two siblings in four cases (17.4%). Around 70% of the children (n=16) attended preschool or day care, and 87% (n=20) had friend/s to play with regularly. About 70% (n=16) were living in a family with an annual household income of more than six million yen, suggesting relatively affluent socioeconomic background.
Table 1 : Characteristics of sample.
Urinary OT levels
(Table 2) OT levels were slightly skewed to the right, with a mean of 179.5 (SD, 78.1; range, 78.5–412.8). The children's urinary OT level was inversely associated with age (r=−0.66, p=0.006), i.e., younger child showed higher urinary OT levels. Further, OT level was higher in boys than in girls (207.3 vs. 143.5 µU/mL per creatinine g/L, t=2.08, p=0.0497). The number of children per household was not significantly associated with OT levels, although those having siblings showed lower OT levels (p=0.12). Having regular friends to play with is significantly associated with lower OT level as opposed to those who do not (158.5 vs. 319.7, t=4.63, p=0.0001). Attendance of preschool or day care and annual household income was not associated with children's urinary OT level.
Table 2 : Urinary oxytocin level for total and subgroup.
Association between demographic characteristics and TTS or SDQ subscales
Older toddlers had more peer problems and fewer prosocial behaviors than younger toddlers; boys had more conduct problems, fewer peer problems, and more prosocial problems than girls; and toddlers having siblings had fewer conduct problems, more hyperactivity, and more peer problems than those without siblings, although not statistically significant.
Pearson's correlations between TTS subscales and OT levels
(Table 3) In the crude model, the attention span and persistence subscale was significantly inversely associated with OT levels (r=−0.52, p=0.011; Figure 1). Further, in partial correlation, the attention span and persistence subscale remained significantly associated with OT levels (r=−0.63, p=0.007). Other TTS subscales (fear, frustration tolerance, regularity, visual and auditory sensitivity and intensity) were not significantly associated with OT levels.
Table 3 : Pearson's correlation between toddler's temperament subscales and urinary oxytocin levels.
Figure 1 : Scatter plot of urinary OT levels, and attention span and persistent temperament score from TTS.
Pearson's correlation between SDQ subscales and OT levels
(Table 4) In the crude model, peer problems were inversely associated (r=−0.60, p=0.002; Figure 2), while prosocial behavior was positively associated (r=0.53, p=0.0097; Figure 2), with OT levels. However, in the partial correlation model, the associations became weaker after the adjustment of demographic characteristics. More specifically, while they remained significant when adjusted for sex and number of siblings (peer problems: r=−0.52, p=0.016; prosocial behavior: r=0.57, p=0.007), they lost their significance after including age (peer problems: r=−0.30, p=0.21; prosocial behavior: r=0.31, p=0.19) and other covariates (peer problems: r=−0.28, p=0.27; prosocial behavior: r=0.21, p=0.42). Other SDQ subscales (emotional symptoms, conduct problems and hyperactivity/ inattention) and total scale as difficult behaviors were not associated with OT.
Table 4 : Pearson's correlation between toddler's SDQ subscales and urinary oxytocin levels.
Figure 2 : Scatter plot of urinary OT levels and prosocial behavior score from SDQ.
Our findings suggest that OT level is associated with toddler's attention span and persistence temperament, although the directionality (i.e., whether low OT induces attention span and persistence temperament or such a temperament is less likely to produce OT) is unknown.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to report on the association between OT levels and toddler's temperament and behavior. It has been reported that children with autism showed lower plasma OT levels [10,11] than children without autism. Our findings are in line with these previous studies, as autistic traits include persistency. Moreover, we have added to the literature that children with higher scores on attention span and persistence, even if not diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder, showed lower OT levels. Although directionality is unknown, it is reasonable to assume that toddlers with longer attention span and persistence temperament are less likely to produce OT as temperament is considered to be innate [25]. The genetic endowment related to attention span and persistence temperament are most likely to be associated with OT related genes, such as oxytocin receptor genes (rs2254298 and rs1042778) or CD38 (rs3796863) [7].
Our results are also consistent with that of previous studies showing the association between OT and prosocial behavior in adults. For example, intranasal OT administration increases both trust and empathy, and improves the capacity to read subtle non-verbal signals allowing individuals to understand the intentions and behavior of social partners [35]. Our study shows that the association between OT and prosocial behavior is found even in toddlers. Thus, intervention such as intensive parental involvement may increase OT levels in toddlers, enhancing prosocial behavior [36]. However, further randomized controlled trials to prove this hypothesis are needed.
Our study showed an inverse association between OT levels and peer problems, while other subscales of SDQ had no association with levels of OT. One possible explanation for this difference is that lower OT levels induce poor social interaction, causing peer problems. A previous study showed that peer problems were significantly inversely correlated with prosocial behavior (r=-0.19, p<0.0001) [33], which is consistent with our results. Meanwhile, the lack of association between OT and emotional problem is inconsistent from the previous study showing the effectiveness of intranasal OT on emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders [37]. In healthy toddlers, association between OT and emotion might be weaker than in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Further study is needed to elucidate how emotion and behaviors interact with OT among healthy children [35].
Several limitations need to be addressed before drawing firm conclusions. First, the participants were recruited through personal connection with one of the authors. This precludes the generalizability of the findings. To lessen the impact of this limitation, our samples were selected from various locations in greater Tokyo and covered a wide range of temperament and social behavior. Second, we used spot urine samples as a proxy of circulating OT levels. However, urinary OT levels may be different from plasma OT, saliva OT (i.e., peripheral OT) or cerebrospinal fluid OT (i.e., central OT) [6,38]. A further study is needed to replicate these findings using other OT measurements to determine whether other measurement methods may yield different results. Nonetheless, in animal models, urinary oxytocin assays have been reported to be a valid biomarker [39]. Third, our cross-sectional design precludes conclusions about the causal relationship between OT levels and temperament or social behavior, particularly if higher OT causes social behavior or vice versa. Also, a toddler's temperament or social behavior can be influenced by parenting during infancy, and parenting for toddlers can be influenced by temperament or social behavior during infancy. Previous longitudinal findings suggested a mutually influencing biobehavioral process where a child's OT levels are shaped by the parent's behavior in early toddlerhood, which is predicted by their OT levels, and the parents' early behavior predicts the reciprocal dialog and empathy with which children make friendly attachments to non-kin members of society [40,41]. Again, a future prospective study is needed to confirm whether circulating OT levels predict social behavior.
In toddlers, attention span and persistence temperament, as well as behavior related to peer problems, were inversely associated with OT levels, while prosocial behavior was positively associated with OT levels. Further longitudinal study is needed to elucidate whether infant temperament determine OT levels, and whether OT levels during infancy determine social behaviors during toddler age.
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions | TF | MO1 | MO2 |
Research concept and design | √ | -- | -- |
Collection and/or assembly of data | -- | -- | √ |
Data analysis and interpretation | √ | -- | -- |
Writing the article | √ | √ | -- |
Critical revision of the article | √ | √ | -- |
Final approval of article | √ | √ | √ |
Statistical analysis | √ | -- | -- |
We thank Ms Hanako Fujiwara, who collected sample from the participants through home visits. We are grateful for all the participants and their family. We would also like to thank Dr. Julian Tang, National Center for Child Health and Development, for proofreading and editing this manuscript. This research was supported by Research Development Grant for Child Health and Development from National Center for Child Health and Development (21 shi -10 and 24-12).
Editors: Stephen R. Bates, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, USA.
Ozgur Duman- Akdeniz University Hospital, Turkey.
Received: 02-Dec-2013 Revised: 08-Jan-2014
Re-Revised: 17-Jan-2014 Accepted: 21-Jan-2014
Published: 29-Jan-2014
Fujiwara T, Ochi M and Osawa M. Association of temperament and social behavior with oxytocin levels among toddlers. Paediatr Health. 2014; 2:2. http://dx.doi.org/10.7243/2052-935X-2-2
Copyright © 2015 Herbert Publications Limited. All rights reserved.